Aposematic Coloration

Mathieu Joron

Leiden University

Insects attract collectors' attention because they are extremely diverse and often bear spectacular colors. To biologists,

FIGURE 1 Pseudosphinx tetrio hawk moth caterpillar from the Peruvian Amazon, showing a combination of red and black, classical colors used by aposematic insects. These larvae feed on toxic latex-sapped trees in the Apocynaceae. Length 14 cm. (Photograph © M. Joron, 1999.)

however, bright coloration has been a constantly renewed puzzle because it makes an insect a highly conspicuous prey to prospective predators. Charles Darwin understood that bright colors or exaggerated morphologies could evolve via sexual selection. However, he felt sexual selection could not account for the conspicuous color pattern of nonreproduc-tive larvae in, for example, Pseudosphinx hawk moth caterpillars (Fig. 1). In a reply to Darwin about this puzzle, Alfred R. Wallace proposed that bright colors could advertise the unpalatability of the caterpillars to experienced predators. Indeed, prey that are not edible to predators are predicted to gain by exhibiting conspicuous and very recognizable colors; experienced predators can then correctly identify and subsequently avoid attacking such prey. E. B. Poulton later developed this idea, expanded it to other warning signals (i.e., sounds or smells), and coined the term "aposematism" to describe this phenomenon (from the Greek "away" and "sign").

Aposematic color patterns are found everywhere throughout the insects, from black- and yellow-striped stinging wasps to black and red, bitter-tasting lady beetles, or brightly colored, poisonous tropical butterflies. Although warning coloration has involved fascination, empirical and theoretical studies for some time, the puzzle of aposematism still motivates much debate today. First, although there is little doubt that bright coloration is often an antipredatory strategy, how aposematism evolves is far from clear. This is because brightly colored mutants in a population of cryptic (camouflaged) prey are more exposed to predators. How can a warning coloration evolve in a prey if the very first mutants exhibiting such coloration in the population are selected against? Second, the reasons for the brightness and conspicuousness of warning colors are not always clear and may be multiple. Are apose-matic colors "road signs" that help predators learn better to differentiate inedible from edible prey, or are bright colors more easily memorized and associated to bad taste by predators? Did yellow and red colors, often borne by poisonous insects, evolve because of innate biases against these colors in the predators' brains, or are more complex cognitive, behavioral, frequency-dependent, or coevolutionary mechanisms involved in the evolution of warning patterns?

Finally, why are warning patterns highly diverse in the insect world, whereas all toxic prey would gain by bearing the same color, thus reducing the probability of being sampled by a naïve predator?

0 0

Post a comment